home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- Animal Action conducted a small ceremony today in Ottawa, Canada,
- marking the death of the first 20 primates killed as funding for Canda's
- monkey colony was cut by the Minister of Health.
- Members of the group planted 20 crosses in the lawn of the center and
- left an offering of fruit next to a picture of one of the monkeys.
-
- Sean Thomas, Co-Director
- Animal Action
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 01:13:39 -0400 (EDT)
- From: OneCheetah@aol.com
- To: ar-news@envirolink.org
- Subject: Re: AR-NEWS digest 469
- Message-ID: <970719011337_-989879526@emout19.mail.aol.com>
-
-
- In a message dated 7/18/97 11:14:41 PM, you wrote:
-
- <<This message is a multi-part MIME message and will be saved with the
- default filename Unknown.mim>>
-
- Arnews digest 469.... couldn't open. Can you re-send?
- OneCheetah
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 13:26:11 -0400
- From: allen schubert <alathome@clark.net>
- To: OneCheetah@aol.com, ar-news@envirolink.org
- Subject: AR-News Admin Note
- Message-ID: <3.0.32.19970719132608.006c8ce0@clark.net>
- Mime-Version: 1.0
- Content-Type: text/plain; charset="us-ascii"
-
- (If you are having technical problems, e-mail me privately.)
-
- Please do not post commentary or personal opinions to AR-News. Such posts
- are not appropriate to AR-News. Appropriate postings to AR-News include:
- posting a news item, requesting information on some event, or responding to
- a request for information. Discussions on AR-News will NOT be allowed and
- we ask that any
- commentary either be taken to AR-Views or to private E-mail.
-
- Continued postings of inappropriate material may result in suspension of
- the poster's subscription to AR-News.
-
- Here is subscription info for AR-Views:
-
- Send e-mail to: listproc@envirolink.org
-
- In text/body of e-mail: subscribe ar-views firstname lastname
-
- Also...here are some websites with info on internet resources for Veg and
- AR interests:
-
- The Global Directory (IVU)
- http://www.veg.org/veg/Orgs/IVU/Internet/netguid1.html
-
- World Guide to Vegetarianism--Internet
- http://www.veg.org/veg/Guide/Internet/index.html
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 13:48:04 +0800 (SST)
- From: Vadivu Govind <kuma@cyberway.com.sg>
- To: ar-news@envirolink.org
- Subject: (MY) One tiger cub dies, another found
- Message-ID: <199707190548.NAA01479@eastgate.cyberway.com.sg>
- Mime-Version: 1.0
- Content-Type: text/plain; charset="us-ascii"
-
-
-
- >The Star Online
- Saturday, July 19, 1997
- Another tiger cub rescued
-
- Locals fear animals being displaced by land
- clearing
-
- KUANTAN: Another starving tiger cub was found in a palm oil plantation
- about 50km here yesterday, after one of two others rescued on Sunday
- died at the Malacca Zoo.
-
- The female cub was seen wandering in the plantation and was captured by
- Indonesian workers about 11.30am. Residents in the Lepar district are now
- worried that massive land clearing for logs at a 3,018ha forested site may
- have displaced a large number of adult tigers, cubs and other wild animals.
-
- There have also been unconfirmed reports that 10 tigers had been shot by
- hunters since the land was cleared.
-
- The timber concession belongs to the Pahang Agriculture Development
- Board (LKPP).
-
- When contacted in Temerloh, Pahang Wildlife and National Park
- Department director Rafeah Muda said he had ordered his officers to
- investigate.
- "At first we thought it was coincidental that two cubs were found but since
- another turned up, we had to investigate," he added.
-
- When asked whether there was illegal hunting going on in the area, Rafeah
- said the department had not received any information on the activity.
-
- The two male cubs were captured by 15 Indonesian workers last Sunday.
-
- Malacca Zoo veterinarian Jawahir Jaafar said one of the dehydrated cubs
- died of pneumonia on Thursday.
-
- The cub was found dead by keepers at 10am.
-
- "A post-mortem showed that it died after the chicken it ate could not be
- digested properly," she said.
-
- She said the cub, which the zoo named 'Alien,' was also under stress,
- probably because it was not used to the zoo environment.
-
- "However, the other cub, Alan, is doing fine and eating well," she said.
-
- When the two four-months old cubs were first placed at the zoo on
- Wednesday, they refused to eat beef but wolfed down pieces of chicken.
-
-
-
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 14:34:38 -0400
- From: allen schubert <alathome@clark.net>
- To: ar-news@envirolink.org
- Subject: (US) McDonald's Lets Libel Case Rest
- Message-ID: <3.0.32.19970719143435.006cca30@clark.net>
- Mime-Version: 1.0
- Content-Type: text/plain; charset="us-ascii"
-
- from AP Wire page:
- -------------------------------------
- 07/18/1997 19:23 EST
-
- McDonald's Lets Libel Case Rest
-
- By DIRK BEVERIDGE
- AP Business Writer
-
- LONDON (AP) -- Despite winning a libel case against them, McDonald's
- Corp. said Friday it won't try to stop two vegetarians from handing out
- leaflets calling the burger chain a menace to animals and its customers.
-
- McDonald's also will not try to recover any of its legal costs, estimated
- at $16 million, or to collect the $98,000 in symbolic damages that it
- won.
-
- The company apparently decided it had had enough in its court brawl
- against unemployed former postman Dave Morris and part-time barmaid Helen
- Steel.
-
- McDonald's defeated them last month when a judge ruled in the 314-day
- ``McLibel'' trial that Morris and Steel had defamed the company by
- handing out pamphlets entitled ``What's wrong with McDonald's? Everything
- they don't want you to know.''
-
- But McDonald's was humiliated when Justice Roger Bell found that several
- key allegations in the anti-capitalist handout were true.
-
- The judge found that McDonald's was responsible for animal cruelty, that
- it exploits children through its ad campaigns and that it pays workers in
- Britain poorly.
-
- McDonald's had until Thursday to file court papers seeking damages, costs
- or a court order to halt publication of the leaflets, but the company let
- the deadline pass with no action.
-
- ``They've obviously realized it's going to be completely futile for them
- and counterproductive,'' said Dan Mills of the McLibel Support Campaign,
- which coordinates publicity for Morris and Steel.
-
- If McDonald's had obtained an injunction to stop Morris and Steel from
- handing out the defamatory pamphlets, the defendants had said they would
- have kept handing them out anyway.
-
- If McDonald's had sought to enforce any court order barring further
- distribution, Morris and Steel could have been jailed for their cause,
- stirring more unwelcome publicity for McDonald's, Mills said.
-
- McDonald's said it took no further action in the case because it had
- already made its point.
-
- ``We've stated all along that we wanted to establish that the allegations
- made against McDonald's weren't true, and we think that's been
- established by the judgment,'' McDonald's spokesman Robert Parker said.
-
- Unfortunately for McDonald's, the fight isn't over.
-
- Steel and Morris plan an appeal later this summer against the portions of
- the three-volume judgment that went against them.
-
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 14:43:38 -0400
- From: allen schubert <alathome@clark.net>
- To: ar-news@envirolink.org
- Subject: (US) Meatpacking Giant's Earnings Fall
- Message-ID: <3.0.32.19970719144336.006bba44@clark.net>
- Mime-Version: 1.0
- Content-Type: text/plain; charset="us-ascii"
-
- (Refs to Mad Cow Disease and E. Coli.....)
- from AP Wire page:
- ---------------------------------
- 07/18/1997 18:45 EST
-
- Meatpacking Giant's Earnings Fall
-
- DAKOTA CITY, Neb. (AP) -- Meatpacking giant IBP Inc.'s second-quarter
- earnings fell 61 percent, the company said Friday, blaming higher cattle
- prices and tight hog supplies.
-
- IBP reported a profit of $33.86 million, or 36 cents per share, on
- revenues of $3.45 billion in the quarter ended June 28. A year earlier,
- the company earned $86.99 million, or 90 cents per share, on revenues of
- $3.26 billion.
-
- The company's stock dropped 12.5 cents per share on the New York Stock
- Exchange to $22.62 1/2 .
-
- ``While our performance during the first six months of this year did not
- match the record-high results of a year ago, we have experienced areas of
- improvement over the last quarter of 1996 and the first quarter of
- 1997,'' said IBP Chairman Robert L. Peterson.
-
- The company said business in the Far East was slowly improving following
- last year's scares over mad cow disease and an outbreak of
- nonmeat-related E. coli in Japan.
-
- The company said the second-quarter earnings reflect the continued
- expense of recent openings of a pork plant at Logansport, Ind., a
- cooked-meats plant at Columbia, S.C., and expanded beef-processing
- operations at Alberta, Canada.
-
- For the first six months of the year, IBP's earned $66.17 million, or 70
- cents per share, down from $140.01 million, or $1.45 per share. Revenues
- rose to $6,58 billion from $6.34 billion.
-
- Date: Sat, 19 Jul 1997 23:00:53 -0400
- From: Vegetarian Resource Center <vrc@tiac.net>
- To: AR-News@envirolink.org
- Subject: Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape
- Message-ID: <3.0.2.32.19970719230053.00d079cc@pop.tiac.net>
- Mime-Version: 1.0
- Content-Type: text/plain; charset="us-ascii"
-
- CHAPTER ONE
-
- Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape
-
- By FRANS DE WAAL
- Photography by FRANS LANTING
-
- University of California Press
-
- Read the Review
-
- The Last Ape
-
- When the lively, penetrating eyes lock with ours and challenge
- us to reveal who we are, we know right away that we are not
- looking at a "mere" animal, but at a creature of considerable
- intellect with a secure sense of its place in the world. We are
- meeting a member of the same tailless, flat-chasted,
- long-armed primate family to which we ourselves and only a
- handful of other species belong. We feel the age-old connection
- before we can stop to think, as people are wont to do, how
- different we are.
-
- Bonobos will not let us indulge in this thought for long: in
- everything they do, they resemble us. A complaining youngster
- will pout his lips like an unhappy child or stretch out an open
- hand to beg for food. In the midst of sexual intercourse, a female
- may squeal with apparent pleasure. And at play, bonobos utter
- coarse laughs when their partners tickle their bellies or armpits.
- There is no escape, we are looking at an animal so akin to
- ourselves that the dividing line is seriously blurred.
-
- Whereas the bonobo amazes and delights many people, the
- implications of its behavior for theories of human evolution are
- sometimes inconvenient. These apes fail to fit traditional
- scenarios, yet they are as close to us as chimpanzees, the
- species on which much ancestral human behavior has been
- modeled. Had bonobos been known earlier, reconstructions of
- human evolution might have emphasized sexual relations,
- equality between males and females, and the origin of the
- family, instead of war, hunting, tool technology, and other
- masculine fortes. Bonobo society seems ruled by the "Make
- Love, Not War" slogan of the 1960s rather than the myth of a
- bloodthirsty killer ape that has dominated textbooks for at least
- three decades.
-
- ARE WE KILLER APES?
-
- In 1925, Raymond Dart announced the discovery of
- Australopithecus africanus, a crucial missing link in the human
- fossil record. This bipedal hominid with apelike features brought
- the human lineage considerably closer to that of the apes than
- previously held possible. It also provided the first indication that
- Charles Darwin had been correct in suggesting Africa, rather
- than Asia or Europe, as the cradle of humanity.
-
- On the basis of evidence encountered at the discovery site, Dart
- speculated that Australopithecus must have been a carnivore
- who ate his prey alive, dismembering them limb from limb,
- slaking his thirst with their warm blood. The killer-ape myth is the
- science writer Robert Ardrey's dramatization of these and other
- ideas, including the proposition that war derives from hunting,
- and that cultural progress is impossible without aggressivity.
- The renowned ethologist Konrad Lorenz added that whereas
- "professional" predators, such as lions and wolves, evolved
- powerful inhibitions keeping them from turning their weaponry
- against their own kind, humans have unfortunately not had time
- to evolve in this direction. Descended from vegetarian
- ancestors, we became meat-eaters almost overnight. As a
- result, our species lacks the appropriate checks and balances
- on intraspecific killing.
-
- It has been suggested that the tremendous appeal of this
- scenario had more to do with the genocide of World War II than
- with fossil finds. Confidence in human nature was at a low after
- the war, and the popularizations of Ardrey and Lorenz merely
- reinforced the misanthropic mood. In A View to a Death in the
- Morning, Matt Cartmill summarizes the impact of the by now
- antiquated idea that the lust to kill has made us what we are:
-
- During the 1960s, the central propositions of the hunting
- hypothesis--that hunting and its selection pressures had
- made men and women out of apelike ancestors, instilled a
- taste for violence in them, estranged them from the animal
- kingdom, and excluded them from the order of
- nature--became familiar themes of the national culture,
- and the picture of Homo sapiens as a mentally
- unbalanced predator, threatening an otherwise
- harmonious natural realm became so pervasive that it
- ceased to provoke comment.... Millions of moviegoers in
- 1968 absorbed Dart's whole theory in one stunning image
- from Stanley Kubrick's film 2001, in which an
- australopithecine who had just used a zebra femur to
- commit the world's first murder hurls the bone gleefully in
- the air--and it turns into an orbiting spacecraft.
-
- Ironically, it is now believed that Australopithecus,
- rather than having been a predator himself, was a
- favorite food for large carnivores. The damage to fossil skulls,
- which Dart interpreted as evidence for club-wielding man-apes,
- turns out to be perfectly consistent with predation by
- leopards and hyenas. In all likelihood, therefore,
- the beginnings of our lineage were marked more by fear
- than ferocity.
-
- BONOBOS AS MODELS
-
- Bonobos are not on their way to becoming human any more
- than we are on our way to becoming like them. Both of us are
- well-established, highly evolved species. We can learn
- something about ourselves from watching bonobos, though,
- because our two species share an ancestor, who is believed to
- have lived a "mere" six million years or so ago. Possibly,
- bonobos have retained traits of this ancestor that we find hard to
- recognize in ourselves, or that we are not used to contemplating
- in an evolutionary light.
-
- Not too long ago, a much more distant relative, the savanna
- baboon, was regarded as the best living model of ancestral
- human behavior. These ground-dwelling primates are adapted
- to the sort of ecological conditions that protohominids must have
- faced after they descended from the trees. The baboon model
- was largely abandoned, however, when it became clear that a
- number of fundamental human characteristics are absent or only
- minimally developed in them, yet present in chimpanzees.
- Cooperative hunting, food-sharing, tool use, power politics, and
- primitive warfare have been observed in chimpanzees, who are
- also capable of learning symbolic communication, such as sign
- language, in the laboratory. Moreover, these apes recognize
- themselves in mirrors--an index of self-awareness for which
- there is thus far little or no evidence in monkeys. Like us, of
- course, chimpanzees belong to the Hominoidea, a branch that
- split off long ago from the rest of the primate tree. They are thus
- genetically much closer to us than are baboons.
-
- Whereas selection of the chimpanzee as the touchstone of
- human evolution represented a great improvement over the
- baboon, one aspect of the models did not need to be adjusted:
- male superiority remained the "natural" state of affairs. In both
- chimpanzees and baboons, males are conspicuously dominant
- over females. In baboons, males are not only twice the size of
- females, they are equipped with canine teeth as formidable as a
- panther's, whereas females lack such weaponry. Sexual
- dimorphism may be less dramatic in the chimpanzee, but in this
- species, too, males reign supreme, and often brutally. It is
- extremely unusual for a fully grown, healthy male chimpanzee to
- be dominated by a female.
-
- Enter the bonobo, which is best characterized as a
- female-centered, egalitarian primate species that substitutes
- sex for aggression. It is impossible to understand the social life
- of this ape without attention to its sex life: the two are
- inseparable. Whereas in most other species, sexual behavior is
- a fairly distinct category, in the bonobo it has become an
- integral part of social relationships, and not just between males
- and females. Bonobos engage in sex in virtually every partner
- combination: male-male, male-female, female-female,
- male-juvenile, female-juvenile, and so on. The frequency of
- sexual contact is also higher than among most other primates.
-
- The bonobo's rate of reproduction is low, however. In the wild, it
- is approximately the same as that of the chimpanzee, with single
- births to a female at intervals of around five years. This
- combination of sexual appetite and slow reproduction sounds
- familiar, of course: nonreproductive sex is a prominent trait of
- our own species.
-
- If the sole purpose of sex is procreation, as some religious
- doctrines would have it, why has the average size of families in
- industrialized nations dropped to fewer than two children,
- despite the fact that countless human couples in those countries
- copulate regularly? Perhaps they do so because it feels good,
- hence tends to become addictive. Yet this automatically raises
- the question: Why does it have this effect on people? After all,
- most other animals restrict their mating activity to a particular
- season or a couple of days in their ovulatory cycles; they do not
- seem to feel any sexual needs divorced from reproduction.
-
- The bonobo, with its varied, almost imaginative, eroticism, may
- help us see sexual relations in a broader context. Certain
- aspects of human sexuality, such as pleasure, love, and
- bonding, tend to be overlooked by reproduction-oriented
- ideologies. The possibility that these aspects have
- characterized our lineage from very early on has serious
- implications, given how often moralizing relies on claims about
- the naturalness or unnaturalness of behavior: what is natural is
- generally equated with what is good and acceptable. The truth is
- that if bonobo behavior provides any hints, very few human
- sexual practices can be dismissed as "unnatural."
-
- Because the role of sex in society is such a loaded and
- controversial issue, scientists have tended to downplay this side
- of bonobo behavior, whereas the few journalists who have
- written about the species have naturally hyped it. In this book, I
- hope to strike a balance: I intend to give the topic the attention it
- deserves, without reducing bonobos to the lustful satyrs that our
- closest relations once were considered to be. Sexual
- encounters of the bonobo kind are strikingly casual, almost
- more affectionate than erotic. If the apes themselves are so
- relaxed about it, it seems inappropriate for us to give in to
- typically human obsessions. In addition, there is a lot more to
- bonobo natural history than sex. The entire social organization of
- the species is fascinating, as is its mode of communication,
- raising of offspring, remarkable intelligence, and status in the
- wild. The whole creature deserves attention, not just part of it.
-
- In the past few years, many different strands of knowledge have
- come together concerning this most enigmatic ape. The findings
- command attention, as the bonobo is just as close to us as its
- sibling species, the chimpanzee. According to DNA analyses,
- we share over 98 percent of our genetic material with each of
- these two apes. And not only are they our nearest relatives; we
- are theirs! That is, the genetic makeup of a chimpanzee or
- bonobo matches ours more closely than that of any other animal,
- including other primates, such as gorillas, traditionally thought of
- as closer to them than to us.
-
- No wonder Carl Linnaeus, who imposed the taxonomic division
- between humans and apes, regretted his decision later in life.
- The distinction is now regarded as wholly artificial. In terms of
- family resemblance, only two options exist: either we are one of
- them or they are one of us.
-
- WHAT'S IN A NAME?
-
- Years ago, when the conservator of mammals at the Amsterdam
- Zoological Museum happened to dust off the stuffed remains of
- an ape named "Mafuca," he immediately recognized its bonobo
- features despite the label, which said it was a chimpanzee.
- During Mafuca's short life, from 1911 through 1916, bonobos
- were not yet recognized as a separate species, even though a
- few keen observers already had an inkling of the difference.
-
- In 1916, a perceptive Dutch naturalist, Anton Portielje,
- speculated in a guide to the Amsterdam Zoo that the hugely
- popular Mafuca might represent a new primate species. A few
- years later, Robert Yerkes, the American pioneer of ape
- research, contrasted "Prince Chim," an individual now known to
- have been a bonobo, with a chimpanzee, noting: "Complete
- descriptions of the physique of the two animals might suggest
- the query as to whether they were both chimpanzees." For all
- intents and purposes, therefore, the species distinction between
- bonobo and chimpanzee ought to be credited to behavioral
- scientists such as Portielje and Yerkes.
-
- It was only when anatomists reached the same conclusion,
- however, that the world paid attention. The distinction, first made
- in 1929, carried tremendous weight: the bonobo became one of
- the last large mammals to be known to science. Rather than in a
- lush African setting, the historic discovery took place in a
- colonial Belgian museum following the inspection of a skull that,
- because it was undersized, was thought to have belonged to a
- juvenile chimpanzee. In immature animals, however, the sutures
- between skull bones ought to be separated, whereas in this
- specimen they were fused. Concluding that it must have
- belonged to an adult with an unusually small head, Ernst
- Schwarz, a German anatomist, declared that he had stumbled
- upon a new subspecies of chimpanzee. Soon the differences
- were considered important enough to elevate the bonobo to the
- status of an entirely new species, officially classified as Pan
- paniscus.
-
- Even though Schwarz's name became officially associated with
- the species--the sort of honor biologists are willing to die for--a
- far more detailed description was provided, in 1933, by Harold
- Coolidge, an American anatomist. Half a century later, Coolidge
- challenged Schwarz's priority. At an international conference of
- primatologists in 1982, he claimed that he himself had been the
- first to notice the unusual skull at the museum. In his excitement
- he had shown it to the museum director, who allegedly told his
- friend Schwarz two weeks later. Schwarz wasted no time
- making the discovery public in an obscure journal published by
- the museum. "I had been taxonomically scooped!" exclaimed
- Coolidge at the symposium. Unfortunately, Schwarz's side to
- this story remains unknown: the accusation came after his
- death.
-
- Oddly enough, the bonobo's genus name, Pan, derives from the
- Greek god of flocks, shepherds, and woods, who had a human
- torso, but the legs, beard, ears, and horns of a goat. Playfully
- lecherous, Pan loved to chase the nymphs and played the
- shepherd's flute, an obvious phallic symbol. The suffix to the
- species name of the bonobo, paniscus, qualifies it as
- diminutive. The other member of the same genus, the
- chimpanzee, carries the species name troglodytes, or cave
- dweller. So we are dealing with rather peculiar epithets for
- animals adapted to the trees, with the bonobo being labeled a
- small herder deity and the chimpanzee a grotto herder deity.
-
- Since the bonobo and chimpanzee are close relatives, and
- since the latter is more familiar, the two species are sometimes
- taken together as two kinds of chimpanzee. Thus, the bonobo is
- also known as the "bonobo chimpanzee" or "pygmy
- chimpanzee." Unfortunately, this usage has forced the name
- "common chimpanzee" upon the chimpanzee--a questionable
- label for an endangered animal. Furthermore, some scientists
- object to "pygmy chimpanzee" as inaccurate (there is
- considerable overlap in size between chimpanzees and
- bonobos), as well as making it sound too much as if the bonobo
- is merely a smaller version of its congener. Others, in turn, say
- the name "bonobo" is meaningless and probably derives from a
- misspelling on a shipping crate of "Bolobo," a town in Zaire.
-
- The label "bonobo" has stuck, though, not least because it
- respects its bearer as a fully distinct species, rather than as, so
- to speak, the poor man's miniature chimp. In addition, "bonobo"
- has a happy ring to it that befits the animal's nature.
- Primatologists acquainted with its behavior have even jokingly
- begun to employ the name as a verb, as in "We're gonna
- bonobo tonight." (The meaning of this expression will be left to
- the reader's imagination!)
-
- To complete these notes on the discovery of the last ape, it has
- recently come to light that, ironically, the bonobo may have been
- known to science longer than any other great ape. The earliest
- accurate description of an ape was produced, in 1641, by
- Nicolaas Tulp, a Dutch anatomist of great repute, immortalized
- in Rembrandt's The Anatomy Lesson. The ape cadaver that Tulp
- dissected resembled a human body so closely in its structural
- details, musculature, organs, and so on, that he commented that
- it would be hard to find one egg more like another. Although Tulp
- baptized his specimen an Indian satyr, adding that the local
- people called it an "orang-outang," it had come straight from
- Africa. Only its name came from the East Indies (in Malay orang
- hutan means "man of the forest").
-
- Tulp's gravure, faithfully replicated over and over in books of the
- seventeenth and eighteenth centuries, appears to show a
- female chimpanzee. At least this was the consensus until a
- British primatologist, Vernon Reynolds, asserted that Tulp's
- satyr could very well have been a bonobo. Reynolds's chief
- argument was that the original drawing shows a cutaneous
- connection between the second and third digits of the ape's
- right foot. Such "webbing" between toes is much more common
- in bonobos than in chimpanzees. Furthermore, Tulp's specimen
- was known to have originated in Angola. Although no bonobos
- live there today, Angola is south of the Zaire River. This
- immense, at times more than one-kilometer-wide water barrier
- currently fully separates chimpanzees, to the north, east, and
- west, from bonobos, to the south.
-
- FIRST IMPRESSIONS
-
- Yerkes greatly admired his bonobo's character and intelligence,
- writing: "I have never met an animal the equal of Prince Chim in
- approach to physical perfection, alertness, adaptability, and
- agreeableness of disposition."
-
- Much has been made of this opinion of one of the greatest
- authorities on ape psychology. Before accepting Yerkes's
- enthusiasm for Chim as a blanket statement about the species,
- however, we should realize that the scientist seriously
- underestimated his subject's age. The slight build of the bonobo
- led him to believe that Chim was only three years old, whereas a
- postmortem inspection by Coolidge indicated an age closer to
- six. In the same way that a child twice the age of another is
- mentally far ahead, Chim may have come across as brilliant
- compared to the chimpanzee, Panzee, with whom he was
- raised. Moreover, Panzee suffered from tuberculosis, another
- serious disadvantage compared to the healthy Chim. Yerkes
- himself fully realized the limitations of his comparison, stating
- that intelligence, temperament, and character very much depend
- on physical constitution.
-
- Unfortunately, these reservations are rarely mentioned when
- Yerkes's high regard for Chim is cited in support of claims that
- bonobos are extraordinarily intelligent. There is no doubt in my
- mind that they are, but whether their intelligence exceeds that of
- other apes remains an open question. Simian IQs are about as
- contentious an issue as human IQs. For one thing, there is great
- individual variability: comparing a few bonobos with a few
- chimpanzees is not going to tell us much. I know some
- exceptionally bright anthropoids, but certainly not all of them are
- bonobos. At this point it is not at all clear in which cognitive
- areas, if any, the bonobo systematically outshines other apes.
-
- The first study of substance comparing bonobos and
- chimpanzees was carried out in the 1930s at the Hellabrunn Zoo
- in Munich. It took Eduard Tratz and Heinz Heck until after World
- War II to publish their findings, based on an inspection of the
- preserved bodies of three apes and film footage collected
- during their lives: terrified by the city's bombardment during the
- war, all three bonobos had died of heart failure. Tratz and
- Heck's eight-point list of behavioral differences between the two
- Pan species still stands as the first outline of the areas of
- greatest contrast: sexual behavior, intensity of aggression, and
- vocal expression. Here follows their list in slightly compressed
- form:
-
- 1. Bonobos are sensitive, lively, and nervous, whereas
- chimpanzees are coarse and hot-tempered.
-
- 2. Bonobos rarely raise their hair; chimpanzees often do
- so.
-
- 3. Physical violence almost never occurs in bonobos, yet
- is common in chimpanzees.
-
- 4. Bonobos defend themselves through aimed kicking with
- their feet, whereas chimpanzees try to pull attackers close
- to bite them.
-
- 5. The bonobo voice contains a and e vowels, whereas
- the chimpanzee uses more u and o vowels.
-
- 6. Bonobos are more vocal than chimpanzees.
-
- 7. Bonobos stretch their arms and shake their hands when
- calling, whereas chimpanzees do not.
-
- 8. Bonobos copulate more hominum and chimpanzees
- more canum.
-
-
- Given what we know now, points 1 through 4 are undoubtedly
- correct. Even though the difference in aggressivity is one of
- degree only, it cannot be denied that the treatment to which
- chimpanzees occasionally subject one another, including biting
- and full-force hitting, is rare among bonobos. Chimpanzees also
- erect their hair at the slightest provocation, pick up a branch,
- and challenge and intimidate anyone perceived as weaker than
- themselves: they are very much into status. By bonobo
- standards, the chimpanzee is a wild and untamed beast, or as
- Tratz and Heck put it: "The bonobo is an extraordinarily
- sensitive, gentle creature, far removed from the demoniacal
- primitive force [Urkraft] of the adult chimpanzee."
-
- As regards point 5, Blanche Learned's pioneer (albeit unwitting)
- comparison of vocal repertoires is worth noting. Before the
- species difference was established, she listened with a musical
- ear to Yerkes's two apes, Chim and Panzee. According to my
- calculations from Learned's phonetic transcriptions of hundreds
- of vocalizations, Chim mostly uttered a (48%), ae (38%), and oo
- (10%) sounds, whereas Panzee mostly uttered oo (68%), o
- (12%), and oa (7%) sounds. There is indeed no quicker way to
- distinguish the two ape species than by their voices. When
- Heck, who was the director of Hellabrunn Zoo, first heard
- bonobo calls coming out of a cloth-covered crate, he was
- convinced that he had received the wrong animals. Their calls
- are so high-pitched and penetrating that they do not even
- remind one of the typical drawn-out "huu ... huu" hooting of the
- chimpanzee. The difference in timbre between the voices of the
- two species may well be of the same magnitude as that
- between a small child and a grown man.
-
- It is also true that bonobos tend to gesticulate when calling, and
- that vocal activity among them is high. Bonobos are excitable
- creatures who frequently "comment" on minor events around
- them through high-pitched peeps and barks. Even if most of
- these vocalizations are noticeable only at close range, one
- definitely hears more vocal exchange in a group of bonobos
- than in a group of chimpanzees. Chimpanzees call when
- seriously alarmed, aroused by food, or in order to intimidate one
- another. Few animals can produce the din characteristic of
- chimpanzees, but much of it occurs on well-circumscribed
- occasions.
-
- The final point concerns sexuality. Because Tratz and Heck
- wrote before the sexual revolution, they felt the need to wrap
- their shocking findings in Latin. In those days, face-to-face
- copulation was regarded as uniquely human; a cultural
- innovation reflecting the dignity and sensibility separating the
- human race from "lower" life forms. The two zoologists claimed,
- however, that whereas chimpanzees mate like dogs (more
- canum), bonobos follow the human pattern (more hominum).
- They added the important observation that the genitals of female
- bonobos seem adapted to this position: the vulva is situated
- between their legs rather than oriented to the back, as is the
- case in chimpanzees.
-
- To this day, both academic and popular writers perpetuate
- ridiculous claims about human mating patterns, penis size, and
- general sexiness. The primary reason for overlooking the
- considerable early knowledge about bonobos must have been
- that most of it was unavailable in English. Who browses through
- journals such as Saugetierkundliche Mitteilungen? Apart from
- their role in the naming game (they were the first to propose
- "bonobo"), Tratz and Heck were ignored and forgotten by the
- scientific community. Another overlooked work is an admirably
- detailed investigation at three European zoos by Claudia
- Jordan, whose 1977 dissertation, "Das Verhalten zoolebender
- Zwergschimpansen" (The Behavior of Zoo-living Pygmy
- Chimpanzees), contains virtually all of the basic behavioral
- information presented as new discoveries in the literature of
- subsequent years.
-
- A second reason that little attention was paid to some of the
- early studies was the tendency to dismiss unusual behavior in
- zoo animals as artifacts of captivity. Could it be that bonobos
- act so grotesquely because they are bored to death, or under
- human influence? We know now that, except under extreme
- conditions, the effects of captivity on behavior are less dramatic
- than used to be assumed. Whatever the conditions under which
- other primates are kept, they never act like bonobos. In other
- words, it must be something in the species, rather than in the
- environment, that produces the bonobo's characteristic
- behavior. It was only when fieldwork got off the ground, however,
- that the behavior-as-artifact explanation could be put to rest.
- Research in the bonobo's natural habitat validated rather than
- contradicted the pioneering observations of Yerkes, Tratz and
- Heck, Learned, Jordan, and others.
-
- In 1974, Alison and Noel Badrian, a young couple of Irish and
- South African extraction, bravely entered the remote jungles of
- northern Zaire on their own, without financial backing. They
- established a study site in Lomako Forest, which is still in use
- today, although observation has been discontinuous and
- conducted by a number of different scientists. The other main
- study site in Zaire, established in the same year, has known
- much greater continuity and has, as a result, become the
- dominant source of information about wild bonobos. This site,
- named Wamba, was founded by Takayoshi Kano of Kyoto
- University, in Japan, after a five-month survey of the distribution
- of Zaire's bonobo population. Transportation by other means
- being virtually impossible in this region, Kano traveled
- enormous distances on foot and by bicycle.
-
- These and other dedicated fieldworkers have advanced our
- knowledge of bonobo behavior by giant strides, confirming the
- significance and richness of these apes' sexual behavior and
- putting their social organization in the context of the ecological
- background to which it is adapted: the swampy rain forest
- covering the flat basin of the Zaire River. Because wild bonobos
- are extremely shy, it takes a long time to habituate them to
- human presence. At Wamba, this problem was solved by a
- technique widely employed with Japanese macaques in the
- investigators' home country: food provisioning. By planting a few
- hectares of sugarcane near their range, Kano was able to entice
- bonobos out of the forest. At Lomako, such techniques have
- never been employed. The Lomako site has therefore
- something unique to offer: a look at the ranging and foraging
- patterns of bonobos undisturbed by human provisioning.
-
- Despite the establishment of Wamba, Lomako, and a handful of
- other field sites, bonobo research still lags far behind that on
- chimpanzees in both scope and intensity. Over recent years,
- however, interest has grown rapidly, not least because bonobos
- seem to present a mirror-image of the traditional picture of our
- primate relatives as male-dominated and violent. As a feminist
- journalist for a nature magazine once put it to me: "Bonobos are
- our only hope!" An ideological interest in the species may not
- sound desirable to most scientists, yet so long as it leads to
- scholarly, honest, and rigorous study, I do not see much wrong
- with it. As a result of continued research, current impressions
- and theories will either be confirmed or require revision, and we
- shall gain a deeper understanding of why bonobos evolved the
- sort of society that they live in.
-
- In the meantime, captive bonobos have become more attractive
- for behavioral studies: zoo colonies now include more
- individuals in more naturalistic enclosures than the single
- individuals or small groups of the past. In addition, the apes live
- longer than before. Bonobos are extremely susceptible to
- respiratory disease: they used to survive only a couple of years
- in captivity. With greater care and better nutrition, there now are
- bonobos aged twenty, thirty, or older in zoos and research
- institutions. The development towards improved survival and
- larger social groupings began at the San Diego Zoo, where I
- conducted my own research. This zoo started out very modestly,
- in the early 1960s, with a single pair of bonobos: Kakowet and
- Linda. These two were so prolific that they produced the
- greatest number of children and grandchildren known of any
- bonobo couple, captive or wild, in the world. Part of the reason
- was that every newborn was taken away to the zoo nursery; this
- allowed Linda to skip the long nursing period and deliver at
- unusually short intervals: ten children in fourteen years.
-
- Not that this is a desirable procedure! Many of Linda's
- newborns were featured on Johnny Carson's late-night television
- show, and I feel they would have been better off with a little less
- fame and a little more motherly love. Nowadays, zoos, including
- the San Diego Zoo, do everything in their power to keep mother
- and infant together.
-
- Linda is still alive (estimated to be around forty, she now lives
- with one of her adult daughters at the Milwaukee County Zoo),
- but Kakowet died years ago. Stories about the patriarch of zoo
- bonobos abound. According to one, Ernst Schwarz was
- overjoyed to hold Kakowet when he was still a small infant (his
- name derives from the French word for peanut, cacahuete,
- because he was so incredibly tiny). Having conducted all of his
- taxonomic work with museum skeletons and skins, Schwarz had
- never met a live bonobo. Standing with the ape on his arm, the
- German anatomist was greeted by a woman who said: "So,
- you're the man who named that funny little monkey." A shocking
- thing to say to someone so familiar with the distinction between
- monkeys and apes!
-
- Now that captive bonobos have become more interesting for
- students of social behavior, and fieldwork is growing in both
- quality and quantity, we are in a better situation than ever before
- to summarize this ape's social life. Our knowledge is far from
- perfect, but we know enough to drag the bonobo out of the
- obscure corner in which primate specialists have been debating
- its peculiarities among themselves. Its behavior is bound to
- overthrow a number of cherished assumptions about the course
- of human evolution. In addition, the species is fascinating in
- itself; it fully deserves a place in the public mind alongside our
- better-known ape relations.
-
- (C) 1997 The Regents of the University of California
- All rights reserved.
- ISBN: 0-520-20535-9
-
-
-
- </pre>
- <!-- END OF PAGE CONTENT -->
-
- </TD>
-
-
- <TD width=50 align=center>
-
- </TD>
- </TR>
-
- <!-- THE BOTTOM TOOLBAR -->
-
- <TR>
-
- <TD colspan=3 align=center fontsize=2>
- <a href="../SUB~1.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/sub.html">ARRS Tools</a> |
- <a href="../NEWSPA~1.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/newspage.html">News</a> |
- <a href="../ORGS~1.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/Orgs.html">Orgs</a> |
- <a href="../SEARCH~1.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/search.html">Search</a> |
- <a href="../SUPPOR~1.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/Support.html">Support</a> |
- <a href="../ABOUT/INDEX.HTM" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/arrs/about">About the ARRS</a> |
- <a href="mailto:arrs@envirolink.org">Contact ARRS</a>
- </TD>
- </TR>
-
-
- <!-- END OF MAIN -->
-
- </TABLE></center>
-
-
-
-
- <!-- THE UNDERWRITERS -->
-
- <table border=0 width=100%>
- <tr><td>
-
- <center> <hr width=285>
- <Font Size=1>THIS SITE UNDERWRITTEN IN PART BY:</FONT>
- <BR>
-
-
- <a href="../../../tppmsgs/msgs22.htm#2206" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/cgi-bin/show_support.pl?id=t891282189&sec=sbn_bottom&url=http%3a//www.outpost.com" target=_top><img src="../../SUPPORT/BANNERS/OTHERS/CYBERIAN/G3.GIF" tppabs="http://www.envirolink.org/support/banners/others/cyberian/g3.gif" border=1 alt="Cyberian Outpost"></a>
-
-
- <hr width=285>
-
- <br><font size=2>
- <b>The views and opinions expressed within this page are not
- necessarily those of the <br>EnviroLink Network nor the Underwriters. The views
- are those of the authors of the work.</b></font>
- </center>
- </td></tr>
-
- </table>
-
- </BODY>
-
- </HTML>
-